Earth Science News
ABOUT US
"Spatial computing" enables flexible working memory
Baking has similar steps - like setting oven temperature and time - but different specifics - like how many degrees and minutes - to remember each time. MIT researchers and colleagues have found evidence that the brain's cortex uses physical space to flexibly remember new specifics while preserving a sense of the overall process. They describe this "spatial computing" idea in a new study.
"Spatial computing" enables flexible working memory
by David Orenstein | Picower Institute for Learning and Memory
Boston MA (SPX) Mar 31, 2023

Routine tasks that require working memory, like baking, involve remembering both some general rules (e.g., read the oven temperature and time from the recipe and then set them on the oven) and some specific content for each instance (e.g., 350 degrees for 45 minutes for a loaf of rye, but 325 degrees for eight minutes for cookies). A new study provides a novel explanation for how the brain distinctly manages the general and specific components of such cognitive demands.

The research, led by scientists at MIT's Picower Institute for Learning and Memory, the Karolinksa Institute, and the KTH Royal Institute of Technology in Stockholm, Sweden, shows that the brain creates distinct spaces in the cortex for each general rule and controls those patches with brain rhythms, a concept the authors call "spatial computing." This system, evident in the study's experiments in animals, explains how the brain can easily sustain a consistent understanding of a process even when the specific contents keep changing (like the time and temperature for bread versus cookies). It also answers a few questions neuroscientists have wrestled with about the physiological operations that underlie working memory.

"Your brain can instantly generalize. If I teach you to follow some rules, like remembering C, A, and B and putting them into alphabetical order, and then I switch the contents to F, D, and E, you wouldn't miss a beat," says Earl K. Miller, Picower Professor in MIT's Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, a member of the Picower Institute for Learning and Memory, and co-senior author of the study in Nature Communications. "Your brain can do this because it represents the rules and the contents at different physical scales. One can just be plugged into the other."

Working memory workings
Years of research by Miller's lab, much of it led by lead author Mikael Lundqvist who is now at Karolinska, have shown that working memory tasks are governed by an interplay of brain rhythms at distinct frequencies. Slower beta waves carry information about task rules and selectively yield to faster gamma waves when it's time to execute operations such as storing information from the senses or reading it out when recall is needed. But these waves operate on networks of millions of neurons, only a smattering of which are actually storing the individual items of information relevant at any particular time. Moreover, neurons that carry information about specific items are found all over the place. Some become more electrically excited, or "spike," in response to different task rules than others, and they often tend to spike at least somewhat even when their information isn't relevant.

So how can these rather imprecise rhythms selectively control just the right neurons at the right times to do the right things? Why are neurons whose spiking relates to specific items scattered and redundant? What makes one neuron that's particular to "350 degrees" perk up when that information has to be stored, but another neuron with that information perk up when it needs to be recalled?

The researchers realized that all these questions could be resolved by the spatial computing theory. Individual neurons representing information items can be scattered widely around the cortex, but the rule that's applied to them is based on the patch of the network they are in. Those patches are determined by the pattern of beta and gamma waves.

"By analyzing a lot of single neurons throughout the years, we had always wondered why so many of them appeared to behave similarly," Lundqvist says. "Regardless of if they preferred the same external stimulus or not, many neurons shared similar patterns of activity during working memory. And these patterns switched from task to task. It also appeared that neurons that were closer together within prefrontal cortex more often shared the same pattern. It started us thinking that memory representations might actually dynamically flow around in prefrontal cortex to implement task rules."

So say your friend calls you at the gym, asking you to retrieve a watch they accidentally left in their locker. This requires turning the padlock dials to the numbers in the combination (e.g., 24, 17, 32). Spatial computing says that when you hear the combination, your brain creates distinct patches for each step (first, second, third). Within each patch the neurons representing the combination number of that particular step become especially excited by gamma waves applied at the time the rule is relevant (i.e., 24 in the "first" patch, 17 in the "second" patch, and 32 in the "third" patch). In this way, individual neurons encoding specific items of information can be selectively associated with general rules by the brain waves controlling the patches they inhabit. In any given patch, all the neurons may be excited somewhat by the gamma waves, but the ones representing the item that fits the rule will spike the most.

"This way memory representations could be dynamically reshaped to fit current task demands independent of how individual neurons are connected or which stimulus they prefer," says co-senior author Pawel Herman of KTH. "It may explain our impressive generalization capabilities in novel situations."

This is not to say that any patch is forever fixed. The patches can come and go for however long they are needed wherever the brain happens to form them for the task at hand. There is no permanent "remember oven temperature" patch in the brain.

"This gives the brain flexibility," Miller said. "Cognition is all about flexibility."

Experimental evidence
The researchers weren't just theorizing. To test spatial computing in real physical brains, they made four experimental predictions about what they should observe as animals played working memory games such as remembering a set of images in an order.

The first prediction was that there should be distinct neural signals about the rules and individual item information. Indeed, the team measured that bursts of waves carried rule information. Individual neural spikes, meanwhile, carried a mix of individual items and task rules, consistent with them representing individual items and having specific rules imposed on them.

The second prediction was that rules information should be spatially organized, and the third prediction was that these rule-enforcing spatial patterns should be consistent so long as the game rules remained the same, regardless of whether the individual items changed. Sure enough, the researchers found that there were different locations for gamma bursts for different rules and that these stayed stable even when the individual items varied during each game.

The final prediction was that the activity of the brain waves should cause neural spiking activity to represent the right information at the right times. This was reflected in the experimental observations, as well. The researchers saw different brain wave patterns for when the brain had to store images in memory and when it had to recall the "right" one. Generally, beta waves were more reduced and neurons spiked more and in a wider area during recall than during storage.

The paper doesn't answer every question about working memory. It's not clear yet how neurons encoding specific information in one patch might be associated with their brethren in another patch, or how the brain controls the patches. More research can answer those further questions about the implications of the new theory of Spatial Computing.

In addition to Miller, Lundqvist, and Herman, the paper's other authors are Scott Brincat, Jonas Rose, Melissa Warden, and Timothy Buschman.

Study funders include the Picower Institute and the JPB Foundation, the European Research Council, the Swedish Research Council, the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, the National Institutes of Health, and the Office of Naval Research.

Research Report:"Working memory control dynamics follow principles of spatial computing"

Related Links
The Picower Institute for Learning and Memory
MIT Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences
All About Human Beings and How We Got To Be Here

Subscribe Free To Our Daily Newsletters
Tweet

RELATED CONTENT
The following news reports may link to other Space Media Network websites.
ABOUT US
Global population could peak below 9 billion in 2050s
New York NY (SPX) Mar 28, 2023
The new projection is significantly lower than several prominent population estimates, including those of the United Nations. The researchers go further to say that if the world takes a "Giant Leap" in investment in economic development, education and health then global population could peak at 8.5 billion people by the middle of the century. The new projections by researchers from the Earth4All initiative for the Global Challenges Foundation is published as a working paper People and Planet, 21st ... read more

ABOUT US
Video probe shows internal damage to Fukushima reactor

Florida lawmakers approve permit-free concealed weapons

White House calls Nashville school shooting 'heartbreaking'

UN's global disaster alert systems goal faces uphill climb

ABOUT US
OpenAI's ChatGPT blocked in Italy: privacy watchdog

Big E3 videogame expo in US is canceled

WVU researchers explore alternative sources to help power space

What can we do about all the plastic waste

ABOUT US
Fire breaks out at Chinese dam construction camp in Pakistan

NGOs slam missed chance to prevent seabed mining

Runoff: not as you might think

UK govt unveils new water plan after sewage discharges outcry

ABOUT US
Deep ocean currents around Antarctica headed for collapse, study finds

Third pole darkening affects local and remote climates

The Greenland ice sheet is close to a melting point of no return

3D radar scan provides clues about threats to iconic Alaskan glacier

ABOUT US
Quake hit one-fifth of Turkey's food production: UN

Fruit in crisis: Florida's orange groves buffeted by hurricane, disease

How Vietnam is trying to stop rice warming the planet

Vietnam's robusta: the go-to coffee bean in a warmer world?

ABOUT US
Malawi's cyclone toll to reach 1,200 as hopes fade

Names Fiona, Ian removed from UN's hurricane roster

Indonesia's Anak Krakatoa volcano erupts, belches huge ash tower

Cyclone Freddy record claim in the eye of the storm

ABOUT US
DNA reveals African and Asian ancestry of medieval Swahili people

Sudan coup leader urges troops to back democratic transition

DR Congo president appoints former militia leader as defence chief

Disinformation attack targeted key Sahel state Niger: AFP investigation

ABOUT US
"Spatial computing" enables flexible working memory

Global population could peak below 9 billion in 2050s

Japanese immigrant's legacy paints Mexico City violet

Vast cemetery in Iraq echoes 14 centuries of life and death

Subscribe Free To Our Daily Newsletters




The content herein, unless otherwise known to be public domain, are Copyright 1995-2024 - Space Media Network. All websites are published in Australia and are solely subject to Australian law and governed by Fair Use principals for news reporting and research purposes. AFP, UPI and IANS news wire stories are copyright Agence France-Presse, United Press International and Indo-Asia News Service. ESA news reports are copyright European Space Agency. All NASA sourced material is public domain. Additional copyrights may apply in whole or part to other bona fide parties. All articles labeled "by Staff Writers" include reports supplied to Space Media Network by industry news wires, PR agencies, corporate press officers and the like. Such articles are individually curated and edited by Space Media Network staff on the basis of the report's information value to our industry and professional readership. Advertising does not imply endorsement, agreement or approval of any opinions, statements or information provided by Space Media Network on any Web page published or hosted by Space Media Network. General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR) Statement Our advertisers use various cookies and the like to deliver the best ad banner available at one time. All network advertising suppliers have GDPR policies (Legitimate Interest) that conform with EU regulations for data collection. By using our websites you consent to cookie based advertising. If you do not agree with this then you must stop using the websites from May 25, 2018. Privacy Statement. Additional information can be found here at About Us.