. | . |
Worms help scientists understand memory formation and recall by Brooks Hays Washington (UPI) Feb 21, 2019 The 302 nerve cells inside roundworms are helping scientists understand why some events or associations can't be remembered. Memory blocking, or Kamin blocking, describes an animal's inability to replace an old memory cue with a new one if the new cue is presented at the same time as the old one. "Suppose you grew up hearing ice cream trucks playing a song and hearing that song, even when you can't see the truck, makes you think of ice cream," Daniel Merritt, professor of molecular genetics at the University of Toronto, said in a news release. "One day, the ice cream truck owners decide to add a spinning green light to the roof of the truck, so that even people who are hard of hearing can see them. Kamin blocking predicts that you won't learn to associate spinning green lights with ice cream, because the ice cream truck song already fully predicts the delicious treat in store for you." Psychologists have pointed to the Kamin blocking as evidence of the importance of novelty for the process of memory formation in humans. While evidence of Kamin blocking can be observed externally among mammals, understanding its neurological underpinnings has proven especially difficult. Worms offer a simpler test model. "Being able to fully describe the molecular changes that are going on in memory is enormously appealing, but human memory is too ephemeral and nebulous to pin it down," said Merritt. "But by studying it in worms, we are really making a lot of headway in figuring out exactly what is going on when memories are formed and retrieved in a molecule by molecule fashion." Before Merritt and his colleagues could study Kamin blocking in worms, they had to confirm the simple roundworm experiences memory blocking. Researchers trained worms to associate hunger with either the taste of salt or the aroma of benzaldehyde, a compound with an almond-like odor. Roundworms typically find the taste of salt and smell of benzaldehyde appealing, but trained worms are repelled by the cues. Next, scientists exposed benzaldehyde-trained worms to both benzaldehyde and salt. When the worms were exposed to only salt, they continued to crawl toward the taste. Salt-trained worms continued to find benzaldehyde appealing, even after being exposed to salt and benzaldehyde together. The first cue prevented the formation of a second association. Next, scientists used green fluorescent molecules to tag and highlight the expression of the EGL-4 protein expressed by a single olfactory nerve cell in the heads of the test subjects. EGL-4 is essential for benzaldehyde starvation learning. When researchers repeated the experiments, they found the EGL-4 protein behaved the same way during benzaldehyde blocking as it did during normal learning. "This is interesting because it contradicts the classic interpretation of blocking where you need an element of surprise or you don't bother remembering the second association," said Merritt. "Our data shows that the memory is formed but it's the expression of behavior that's suppressed somehow." Scientists shared the latest findings in the journal Scientific Reports. Blocking's effects last four hours, preventing a new association from being learned. Scientists don't know why blocking produces a hangover effect. "That's the big question," said Merritt. "I'll let you know in four years' time when I'm done with my PhD."
Acacia ants' vibrational sensors can differentiate between nibbles and wind Washington (UPI) Feb 15, 2019 Acacia ants have an exceptional sense of vibration. As a new study revealed, the ants, which live in and on Africa's acacia trees, can tell the difference between the vibrational patterns made by wind rustling the tree's leaves and those made by a munching herbivore. The ants aren't afraid of being eaten themselves. Instead, the vibrations trigger ants to patrol the branches of the tree and fend off animals looking to eat the tree's leaves and bark. In return for the ants' defensive efforts, th ... read more
|
|
The content herein, unless otherwise known to be public domain, are Copyright 1995-2024 - Space Media Network. All websites are published in Australia and are solely subject to Australian law and governed by Fair Use principals for news reporting and research purposes. AFP, UPI and IANS news wire stories are copyright Agence France-Presse, United Press International and Indo-Asia News Service. ESA news reports are copyright European Space Agency. All NASA sourced material is public domain. Additional copyrights may apply in whole or part to other bona fide parties. All articles labeled "by Staff Writers" include reports supplied to Space Media Network by industry news wires, PR agencies, corporate press officers and the like. Such articles are individually curated and edited by Space Media Network staff on the basis of the report's information value to our industry and professional readership. Advertising does not imply endorsement, agreement or approval of any opinions, statements or information provided by Space Media Network on any Web page published or hosted by Space Media Network. General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR) Statement Our advertisers use various cookies and the like to deliver the best ad banner available at one time. All network advertising suppliers have GDPR policies (Legitimate Interest) that conform with EU regulations for data collection. By using our websites you consent to cookie based advertising. If you do not agree with this then you must stop using the websites from May 25, 2018. Privacy Statement. Additional information can be found here at About Us. |