|
. | . |
|
by Staff Writers St. Louis MO (SPX) Aug 05, 2015
The more than 200 species in the family Mormyridae communicate with one another in a way completely alien to our species: by means of electric discharges generated by an organ in their tails. In a 2011 article in Science that described a group of mormyrids able to perceive subtle variations in the waveform of electric signals, Washington University in St. Louis biologist Bruce Carlson, PhD, noted that another group of mormyrids are much less discriminating. The fish with nuanced signal discrimination can glean a stunning amount of information from electric signals, including the signaler's species, sex, age, relative dominance status, and possibly even individual identity. They can also detect emotional states, such as aggression, submission, courtship and active exploration (see video). The fish with the simpler communication system were less studied and less well understood. In the August 4 issues of the online journal eLife, Carlson and graduate student Christa Baker describe how they discovered the basis for the perceptual differences between the two groups of fish by studying the fish's sensory receptors. "There had to be a neural correlate for the perceptual differences, so we looked to see if something was happening 'out in the periphery' where the signals are originally detected and encoded for processing in the brain," Carlson said. The receptors in the less discriminating fish encode signals very differently than do the receptors in the more discriminating fish. Further, the receptors of the less discriminating fish are tuned to the collective signals from schools or shoals of fish rather than to those from individual fish. "As far as we know, this is the first time anyone has found a receptor tuned to group communication signals rather than those coming from individuals," Carlson said.
Spiking versus oscillating "We knew from work in the 1960s that there were differences in the physiology, or electrical behavior, of the sensory receptors," Baker said. "The broadly distributed receptors fire spikes, or action potentials, whereas the clustered receptors produce oscillating potentials at a constant frequency." "We learned that when the oscillating recepters receive an electric signal, they reset their oscillation to a particular point in the cycle," she said. "This phase reset briefly syncrhonizes the oscillations of different receptors." "No one had ever described anything like this in a sensory system before," Carlson said. "This is the first sensory receptor we know of that encodes stimuli by resetting the phase of ongoing oscillations." "We investigated how the phase reset varied with different stimuli," Baker said, "and we found that the oscillating receptors do not encode the same information as spiking receptors." The spiking receptors, which fire a nerve impulse every time there is an upward or downward excursion in the signal, are very good at encoding precise timing cues in signaling waveforms. But oscillating receptors encode only the onset of the signal and its point of origin. "Fish with oscillating receptors are not behaviorally sensitive to waveform variation at least in part because the precise timing cues are not encoded by their receptors," Baker said.
Group think The spiking receptors are maximally sensitive to frequencies near those that make up the signals from other members of their species. The oscillating receptors, however, are tuned to much lower frequencies than those that characterize signals from their fellow fish. "We found that the timing patterns they were most sensitive to were patterns that can only be produced by the collective signaling of large groups of fish," Baker said. "We think that the oscillating receptors help fish detect and locate those groups." But why are these two groups of fish so different? "Our best guess is that differences in social behavior, in social organization, selected for differences in sensory capacities," Carlson said. "Based on what I've seen in the field," he said, "the fish able to perceive small differences in the communication signals tend to be solitary and territorial, whereas the fish whose receptors are tuned to group signals tend to be more gregarious. "My guess is that these two different lifestyles place very different selective pressures on communication. If you're solitary and territorial and you detect another fish in the area, you want to know exactly who that fish is. Is it a potential competitor, a potential mate, or a different species you're not worried about? "On the other hand, if you're living in a shoal or school of fish, it's not so important to identify individuals. Just sticking with the group is going to be a successful strategy."
Related Links Washington University in St. Louis Darwin Today At TerraDaily.com
|
|
The content herein, unless otherwise known to be public domain, are Copyright 1995-2014 - Space Media Network. All websites are published in Australia and are solely subject to Australian law and governed by Fair Use principals for news reporting and research purposes. AFP, UPI and IANS news wire stories are copyright Agence France-Presse, United Press International and Indo-Asia News Service. ESA news reports are copyright European Space Agency. All NASA sourced material is public domain. Additional copyrights may apply in whole or part to other bona fide parties. Advertising does not imply endorsement, agreement or approval of any opinions, statements or information provided by Space Media Network on any Web page published or hosted by Space Media Network. Privacy Statement All images and articles appearing on Space Media Network have been edited or digitally altered in some way. Any requests to remove copyright material will be acted upon in a timely and appropriate manner. Any attempt to extort money from Space Media Network will be ignored and reported to Australian Law Enforcement Agencies as a potential case of financial fraud involving the use of a telephonic carriage device or postal service. |